Archaeology: What Is It?
The Feature System
Mapping the Site
Stratigraphy
HOW DO WE DO IT?
HOW COME WE DO IT THAT WAY?
HUH - HOW COME? IS THAT RIGHT?


Archaeology Defined: Goals and Frameworks
Archaeology is simply the study of the material things of the past.There is over a century of jargon that can and will be applied, but in very simplest terms, archaeologists study stuff that is preserved so that we may decipher something of the culture, beliefs, and values of past societies. Archaeology is ANTHROPOLOGY, not really a subdiscipline, but perhaps a body of methods and teachniques that are applied to the analysis of MATERIAL CULTURE (cf. Gardin and Peebles 1992; Leone and Potter 1988; Malina and Vasicek 1990; Renfrew and Zubrow 1994; Tilley 1990). Material culture is that whole domain of things made and used by people. Material culture studies embody analyses of technology, design, function, social organization, history, religion, ritual and belief. People use things to encode and inculcate behavior, to express values and feelings, as well as to extract energy and meet nutritional requirements. Archaeologists expect to be able to find out a great deal about past societies through careful systematic analyses of the objects those societies created. The record of the past is most often incomplete. Many objects are perishable, and they simply are not preserved in archaeological sites. Patterns in artifacts are found, however, and this patterning preserves much of the original context of use and meaning that surrounded the creation and use of every object.

Archaeology's history is a checkered one (Trigger 1989; Willey and Sabloff 1993). The first archaeologists were avocational explorers, diggers, and speculators. Some were fascinated by holding bits of the past. Others were driven by the desire to make money off of exotic treasures. This was the era of Indian Jones. Archaeologists were active fieldworkers, certainly better than theorizing anthropologists permanently glued to armchairs in upper class Victorian sitting rooms, but they were destructive collectors who intended to amass neat objects for display in museums.

By the middle 1900s, archaeologists began to try to preserve more information about the things they found. Elaborate strategies were designed for classification of things. Complete descriptions were the goal, and archaeology became more and more systematic, as well as more and more scientific in view and approach. This was the rise of the CULTURE-HISTORICAL SCHOOL of archaeology in the United States. Things were carefully excavated to standards of stratigraphic recording and reports emphasized the location and dating of things. Digs, often facilitated through Works Projects of America funding, were massive in scale. Entire sites were excavated and collections of things numbered in the hundreds of thousands.

Construction of local and regional chronologies was the goal, and distributions of objects in SPACE and TIME were held indicative of past societies and the movements of cultures and cultural ideas across the landscape (cf. Willey and Phillips 1958). Cultural TRAITS were defined that had demonstrated importance in defining the stringers of time and space, and these became the major rubrics for analysis and discussion of archaeological CULTURES. Emphasis was on description, CLASSIFICATION, and construction of narratives that explained the ARCHAEOLOGICAL RECORD in terms of alleged cultures that moved through space of time. Changes in traits became axiomatically associated with changes in basic elements of socioeconomic organization and earmarks of changing adaptive systems as well as labels for specific ethnic and linguistic groups in the past.

WHAM - BAM! CAME THE 60s! Old traditional culture history was bashed, mashed, chewed, and abused by the NEW ARCHAEOLOGISTS. These Mad Dog-, hell-bent-for-science PROCESSUALISTS would truck no more lengthy descriptions of stuff and tedious typological constructions (Binford 1962, 1964, 1965, 1968; Leone 1972; Watson et al. 1971). Their rallying cry was that there must be more to archaeology than a slavish adherence to the old kulturkreise school. Processualists would emphasize use of basic scientific method, with reliance on explicit HYPOTHESIS TESTING and characterization of human societies as ADAPTIVE SYSTEMS (e.g., Binford and Sabloff 1982; Clarke 1977; Flannery 1967, 1968, 1982; Hodder 1978). Culture-historical reconstructions were declared to be pseudo explanatory frameworks and definition of traits though useful for placing past societies in the basic framework of time and space could not be the declared end-products of archaeological research. CULTURAL CHRONOLOGIES were simply the beginning, serving as useful classificatory frameworks, in which more compelling questions of adaptation and CULTURAL CHANGE could be MODELED and addressed (cf. Clarke 1978; Sabloff 1981). The archaeological record was seen as archaeology's laboratory for studying HUMAN BEHAVIOR over the long expanse of time. Processual archaeology was firmly based in CULTURAL MATERIALISM in the mode of Leslie White and Marvin Harris, and asserted that the only suitable approach was to focus on the relationships between society and the physical environment. TECHNOLOGY was the principal means of human adaptation. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION facilitated the extraction of resources through use of technology. IDEOLOGY simply justified what had to be done to accomodate production (Harris 1968, 1979; White 1959, 1969, 1975). This view holds that cultures are inherently or overwhelmingly rational: CULTURE is defined as man's extrasomatic means of adaptation to his environment. Proponents asserted that archaeology was uniquely suited to establish patterns in how humans chose to adapt to their physical environments, and that archaeologists would contribute to knowledge of human behavior by identifying UNIVERSALS that would address behavior in terms of past-present-future.

In the early '80s a group of pragmatic archaeologists, leery of dooming culture-historical reconstructions to the trash heap and appalled by POSITIVIST materialist dogmas, advocated a HOLISTIC inspection of all facets of culture (technology, social organization, ideology) by archaeologists. These anthropologists were tagged as POST-PROCESSUALISTS (e.g., Hodder 1982, 1986, 1993; Shanks and Tilley 1987; Tilley 1990). This label actually encompasses a wide range of anthropological approaches that are grouped only when considered in contrast to the more narrowly defined materialist approach of the processualists. Post-Processualists emphasize study of patterns in material culture and insist that archaeologists can extract meaningful inferences about values, beliefs, religion, and social structure, as well as the more obvious socioeconomic or technological elements of human adaptation.

Archaeology for Post-Processualists is often defined as the anthropology of material culture, and encompasses but is not limited to Materialist positivistic approaches to study of past societies. Key words for Post-Processual approaches include material culture study, with key concepts being CONTEXT and CRITICAL THEORY. As the anthropology of things, Post-Processualist theory draws upon a vast array of anthropological theories, including Structuralism, Symbolic Anthropology, and tenets of Critical Marxism. Archaeology emerges in its P-P guise as a social anthropology of the past, not purely Western Science and not purely History, but a holistic study of humans in the past, including their institutions, their beliefs, and their culture in all its many facets.

Post-Processual approaches build on Culture History and on the New Archaeology of the Processualist school, but seek to broaden the field of study dramatically. P-p archaeologists are anthropologists who study material culture (Past-Present-Future). Under this definition, archaeologists no longer have to excavate sites. They might explore dumps, analyze gravestones, perform surveys of extant architecture, or canvas wearing apparel in shopping malls. P-P types dig though, and the analytical framework presented here addresses the tenets of the old school and applies method of the new.

Terms and Concepts
Archaeology as a discipline predicated on the analysis of a complex subject, has evolved a jargon or lexicon all its own. In these cartoons, reproduced from Malina and Vasicek (1990:Figs.3,4), the archaeologist attempts to sort out all the myriad jargon terms that have become shorthand referents to theories and concepts. All deal with what the archaeologist finds, and how the archaeologist constructs inferences.
                
Figure 1. Cartoons showing the jargonized process of description and classification: a task not for the meek (from Malina and Vasicek 1990: Figs. 3 and 4).

ARTIFACTS are things made, used, and modified by people. The term refers to tools, residues of tool manufacture, residues of processing plants and animals, and by-products of tool manufacture, processing, or other human activities.

NATUREFACTS are parts of plants or animals not modified by man, but indicative of man's physical environment. For example, seeds, plant parts or small animal remains found in an archaeological site can be analyzed to determine season of the year for man's occupation or indicate the climate and environment of a given period.

MENTEFACTS are the ideas and values that govern the creation of artifacts. Also called "mental templates" or "cognitive maps" or "cognitive frameworks," these structures are not beyond archaeological inference. Recognition of Mentefacts allows archaeologists to explore nonmaterial aspects of culture through analysis of the material manifestations of cultural needs and beliefs.

How Do We Get To Inference from Things?
These cartoons depict the inner recesses of the researcher's mind: things found, categories applied, inferences drawn, facts elicited. Artifact applies to the totality of the object or thing found. To find patterns indicative of human behavior, and hence, culture, we must have a finer level of measurement: the ATTRIBUTE. Attributes are measurements or observations drawn or about artifacts. The attribute measured may be one zone of wear on a formed tool. So, a given tool form may have dozens of measurable attributes. No description is adequate without measurements taken at the attribute level of analysis. Attributes may record basic morphology, alterations of surfaces, signposts of manufacture, indicators of depositional environment, quantities or requencies, and any number of other variable dimensions. Malina and Vasicek (1990), Neustupny (1993) and Shennan (1988) offer excellent discussions of the need for rigorous measurement frameworks in archaeological research (see also Clarke 1968, 1972 and 1977).

Inferences about past human behavior are constructed by correlating and cross-tabulating attribute lists. Our inferential patterns are then arranged in shifting hierarchies, with one form or focus simply being the level of measurement at which the pattern analysis is performed:

Gross: distribution of artifact types within the site.

Moderate: patterns of selected features observed on artifacts.

Fine: patterns defined by careful correlations of descriptive dimensions outlining attribute characteristics across tool types.

Very Fine: microscopic examination of working edges and working surfaces and physical and chemical examination of surfaces and residues, which result in pattern analysis performed on parts of a single artifact. Here, the artifact becomes directly analogous to the archaeological site.
References Cited

Binford, L.R.

Binford, L.R.

Binford, L.R.

Binford, L.R.

Binford, L.R. and J.A. Sabloff

Clarke, D.L.

Clarke, D.L.

1972 Models in Archaeology. ed. London: Methuen.

1977 Spatial Archaeology. London: Academic Press.

Flannery, K.V.

Flannery, K.V.

Flannery, K.V.

Gardin, J.-C. and C.S. Peebles (eds.)

Harris, M.

Harris, M.

Hodder, I.

Hodder, I.

Hodder, I.

Hodder, I.

Leone, M.P. (ed.)

Leone, M.P. and P.B. Potter (eds.)

Malina, J. and Z. Vasicek

Neustupny, E.

Renfrew, C. and E.B. Zubrow (eds.)

Sabloff, J.A. (ed.)

Shanks, M. and C. Tilley

Shennan, S.

Tilley, C. (ed.)

Tilley, C. (Ed.)

Watson, P.J., S.A. LeBlanc and C.L. Redman

White, L.A.

White, L.A.

White, L.A.

Willey, G.R. and P. Phillips

Willey, G.R. and J.A. Sabloff


Concepts to Review